The Theory of Imprinting - Its Implications and Ramifications in Raptors and All Birds

Abstract

There is a growing body of evidence indicating that for young birds of many species, including most raptors, early social experience plays a major role in their subsequent sexual preferences. This socialization experience, often referred to as sexual imprinting, has critical implications for captive breeding programs, including those involving the handrearing of young by human caretakers, and those involving foster rearing of chicks by parents of closely related species. For example, it has been shown that when young birds of a variety of species are foster reared through the time of fledging, they will subsequently direct their sexual preferences toward the foster species. Further, this preference is often irreversible. That is, even large amounts of post-fledging exposure to their own species may not reverse their preference for their foster species. The amount of exposure needed for this imprinting to occur, when this exposure need occur, how irreversible this imprinting is, and whether males and females show the same effect from this experience, may vary from species to species.

Sexual imprinting may be defined as a learning process by which a young bird learns the features of the stimulus (usually members of its own species) to which it will subsequently direct its sexual behavior upon reaching sexual maturity. The learning takes place within a sensitive period which typically ends prior to sexual maturity. In altricial birds such as raptors, this sensitive period ends about the time of fledging. The learning is generally considered to be irreversible. That is, sexual preferences formed during the sensitive period are difficult if not impossible to reverse. Note that irreversibility refers to a preference. In other words, if a foster imprinted bird is placed with only conspecifics, it may successfully mate with them, however, if given the choice between its own species and its foster species, it will choose to mate with its foster species. This issue is of particular relevance to captive breeding programs with raptors, A raptor imprinted on humans might fail to mate with conspecifics when humans are present, but might successfully mate with conspecifics if their human caretakers avoided contact with them. The learning is generally thought to be supraindividual. Bateson (1966) states that sexual imprinting, in fact, involves individual learning, that birds learn the features of its close kin so that it will subsequently mate with similar individuals.

In 1935, Konrad Lorenz, in a paper that was to become a milestone in the study of behavior, emphasized that in some birds the preference of an individual to mate with a member of its own species is subject to learning shortly after hatching. In some species, if a bird is raised by a foster mother, either another species of bird or a human keeper, the bird will become socially attached to the foster mother and, on reaching sexual maturity, will court individuals of the foster mother's species in preference to those of its own species. As examples, Lorenz cited studies of his own and others on ducks, geese, owls, parrots, herons and other birds, including both precocial and altricial species.

The process by which these important and long lasting first social bonds were formed is termed imprinting. To distinguish the early learning of preferences that are tested by the following response to mother objects and the early learning of preferences that are tested in courtship, the former will be referred to as filial imprinting, and the latter as sexual imprinting. Warriner (1963) showed that choice of a mate within a species can be largely determined by early experience, but his work only dealt

with color variants within one species so it did not test the potency of early learning as an isolating

· mechanism.

This problem was then studied by Schutz (1965, 1971) in waterfowl which showed that young raised by foster parents and then exposed to their own species subsequently tried to pair with the species they had been raised with. The effectiveness of an imprinting experience is partly determined by other predisposing factors (presumably genetic) which remain to be explored.

Probably the most enduring of the characteristics of imprinting pointed out by Hess (1959, 1964) was the observation that a social attachment by the young to an object occurs most easily, and perhaps mainly, during a relatively short period just after hatching. This became known as the critical period, or sensitive period, for imprinting. This optimum age is defined by an increase in imprintability as the animal nears it, and a decrease afterwards.

Brosset (1971), in an extensive investigation of cross fostering in doves, discovered that a majority of the cross fostered young oriented their sexual behavior toward the foster species. Immelmann (1969, 1972), in studies with cross fostering in finches, showed that most of the offspring directed their courtship exclusively or preferentially to their fostering species.

The advantages of imprinting raptors on man have been realized by many for years. Imprinted raptors can be used for educational purposes and research but, perhaps, the best application is captive breeding. Raptors can be trained to copulate with their handlers upon reaching sexual maturity. The social interaction necessary to keep this type of pair bond intact is time consuming but essential. Daily contact either in the mews or by using falconry techniques will keep this pair bond intact. Falconry techniques assure the proper interaction with humans to continue the pair bond. Pair bond maintenance behaviors such as feeding, preening, etc., can be better maintained if the bird is kept under such conditions.

 

 

 

PDF

References

Bateson, P.P.G., 1966. The Characteristics and Context of Imprinting, Biol. Rev. Cambridge Phil. Soc. 41: 177-220.

Brosset, A., 1971. Imprinting. Chez Les Colombides-Etude Des Modifications Comportamentales Aucours de Vieillisement. Z. Tierpsychol. 29:279-300.

Hess, E.H., 1959. Imprinting. Science 130: 133-141.

--- 1964. Imprinting in Birds, Science 146: 1128-1139.

Immelmann, K., 1969. Song Development in the Zebra Finch and Other Estrildid Finches, pp. 61-74. In R.A. Hinde, ed., Bird Vocalizations, Cambridge Univ., Press, London.

Lorenz, K., 1935. Companions as Factors in the Bird's Environment. Ornithol. 83:137-213, 289-413.

Warriner, C.C., W.B. Lemmon and T.S. Ray, 1963. Early Experience as a Variable in Mate Selection. Animal Behav. 11:221-224.

Weaver, J.D., T.J. Cade, 1983. Falcon Propagation, A Manual on Captive Breeding. The Peregrine Fund, Inc. •